Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 5.858
Filtrar
1.
J Vis ; 24(4): 20, 2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38656530

RESUMEN

We obtain large amounts of external information through our eyes, a process often considered analogous to picture mapping onto a camera lens. However, our eyes are never as still as a camera lens, with saccades occurring between fixations and microsaccades occurring within a fixation. Although saccades are agreed to be functional for information sampling in visual perception, it remains unknown if microsaccades have a similar function when eye movement is restricted. Here, we demonstrated that saccades and microsaccades share common spatiotemporal structures in viewing visual objects. Twenty-seven adults viewed faces and houses in free-viewing and fixation-controlled conditions. Both saccades and microsaccades showed distinctive spatiotemporal patterns between face and house viewing that could be discriminated by pattern classifications. The classifications based on saccades and microsaccades could also be mutually generalized. Importantly, individuals who showed more distinctive saccadic patterns between faces and houses also showed more distinctive microsaccadic patterns. Moreover, saccades and microsaccades showed a higher structure similarity for face viewing than house viewing and a common orienting preference for the eye region over the mouth region. These findings suggested a common oculomotor program that is used to optimize information sampling during visual object perception.


Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular , Movimientos Sacádicos , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología
2.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 9433, 2024 04 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38658592

RESUMEN

Selective retrieval of context-relevant memories is critical for animal survival. A behavioral index that captures its dynamic nature in real time is necessary to investigate this retrieval process. Here, we found a bias in eye gaze towards the locations previously associated with individual objects during retrieval. Participants learned two locations associated with each visual object and recalled one of them indicated by a contextual cue in the following days. Before the contextual cue presentation, participants often gazed at both locations associated with the given object on the background screen (look-at-both), and the frequency of look-at-both gaze pattern increased as learning progressed. Following the cue presentation, their gaze shifted toward the context-appropriate location. Interestingly, participants showed a higher accuracy of memory retrieval in trials where they gazed at both object-associated locations, implying functional advantage of the look-at-both gaze patterns. Our findings indicate that naturalistic eye movements reflect the dynamic process of memory retrieval and selection, highlighting the potential of eye gaze as an indicator for studying these cognitive processes.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Fijación Ocular , Recuerdo Mental , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Adulto , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Memoria/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología
3.
J Vis ; 24(4): 23, 2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38662346

RESUMEN

This paper reviews projection models and their perception in realistic pictures, and proposes hypotheses for three-dimensional (3D) shape and space perception in pictures. In these hypotheses, eye fixations, and foveal vision play a central role. Many past theories and experimental studies focus solely on linear perspective. Yet, these theories fail to explain many important perceptual phenomena, including the effectiveness of nonlinear projections. Indeed, few classical paintings strictly obey linear perspective, nor do the best distortion-avoidance techniques for wide-angle computational photography. The hypotheses here employ a two-stage model for 3D human vision. When viewing a picture, the first stage perceives 3D shape for the current gaze. Each fixation has its own perspective projection, but, owing to the nature of foveal and peripheral vision, shape information is obtained primarily for a small region of the picture around the fixation. As a viewer moves their eyes, the second stage continually integrates some of the per-gaze information into an overall interpretation of a picture. The interpretation need not be geometrically stable or consistent over time. It is argued that this framework could explain many disparate pictorial phenomena, including different projection styles throughout art history and computational photography, while being consistent with the constraints of human 3D vision. The paper reviews open questions and suggests new studies to explore these hypotheses.


Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Fóvea Central/fisiología
4.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38554119

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: Although research has shown that the replay of encoding-specific gaze patterns during retrieval, known as gaze reinstatement, facilitates memory retrieval, little is known about whether it differentially associates with the negativity preference in memory (defined as enhanced memory for negative stimuli relative to neutral stimuli in this study) among younger and older adults. The present study aims to address this research gap. METHODS: A total of 33 older adults (16 women; aged 58-69 years, M = 63.48, SD = 2.98) and 36 younger adults (10 women; aged 18-26 years, M = 20.39, SD = 1.57) completed a remember/know recognition memory task involving negative and neutral pictures. Their eye movements were tracked during both the memory encoding and retrieval phases. RESULTS: Younger and older adults had better memory for negative than neutral pictures. Older adults exhibited significantly stronger gaze reinstatement for negative than neutral stimuli, while this difference was nonsignificant in younger adults. Moreover, gaze reinstatement is positively linked to memory performance in both age groups. DISCUSSION: The results suggest that gaze reinstatement may play age-differential roles in the negativity preference of memory. Negative valence may enhance gaze reinstatement, which improves subsequent recognition memory, particularly among older adults. The finding contributes to a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying the negative preference for memory in different age groups.


Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Anciano , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Envejecimiento/fisiología
5.
Nature ; 623(7986): 381-386, 2023 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37880369

RESUMEN

To maintain a stable and clear image of the world, our eyes reflexively follow the direction in which a visual scene is moving. Such gaze-stabilization mechanisms reduce image blur as we move in the environment. In non-primate mammals, this behaviour is initiated by retinal output neurons called ON-type direction-selective ganglion cells (ON-DSGCs), which detect the direction of image motion and transmit signals to brainstem nuclei that drive compensatory eye movements1. However, ON-DSGCs have not yet been identified in the retina of primates, raising the possibility that this reflex is mediated by cortical visual areas. Here we mined single-cell RNA transcriptomic data from primate retina to identify a candidate ON-DSGC. We then combined two-photon calcium imaging, molecular identification and morphological analysis to reveal a population of ON-DSGCs in the macaque retina. The morphology, molecular signature and GABA (γ-aminobutyric acid)-dependent mechanisms that underlie direction selectivity in primate ON-DSGCs are highly conserved with those in other mammals. We further identify a candidate ON-DSGC in human retina. The presence of ON-DSGCs in primates highlights the need to examine the contribution of subcortical retinal mechanisms to normal and aberrant gaze stabilization in the developing and mature visual system.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Macaca , Retina , Células Ganglionares de la Retina , Animales , Humanos , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Retina/citología , Retina/fisiología , Células Ganglionares de la Retina/citología , Células Ganglionares de la Retina/fisiología , Movimiento (Física) , Análisis de Expresión Génica de una Sola Célula , Ácido gamma-Aminobutírico/metabolismo , Señalización del Calcio , Fijación Ocular/fisiología
6.
Behav Res Ther ; 165: 104311, 2023 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37037182

RESUMEN

Bilateral eye movement (EM) is a critical component in eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR), an effective treatment for post-traumatic stress disorder. However, the role of bilateral EM in alleviating trauma-related symptoms is unclear. Here we hypothesize that bilateral EM selectively disrupts the perceptual representation of traumatic memories. We used the trauma film paradigm as an analog for trauma experience. Nonclinical participants viewed trauma films followed by a bilateral EM intervention or a static Fixation period as a control. Perceptual and semantic memories for the film were assessed with different measures. Results showed a significant decrease in perceptual memory recognition shortly after the EM intervention and subsequently in the frequency and vividness of film-related memory intrusions across one week, relative to the Fixation condition. The EM intervention did not affect the explicit recognition of semantic memories, suggesting a dissociation between perceptual and semantic memory disruption. Furthermore, the EM intervention effectively reduced psychophysiological affective responses, including the skin conductance response and pupil size, to film scenes and subjective affective ratings of film-related intrusions. Together, bilateral EMs effectively reduce the perceptual representation and affective response of trauma-related memories. Further theoretical developments are needed to elucidate the mechanism of bilateral EMs in trauma treatment.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Memoria , Trauma Psicológico , Percepción Visual , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Trauma Psicológico/fisiopatología , Humanos , Afecto , Masculino , Femenino , Adolescente , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Autoinforme , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Emociones , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Desensibilización y Reprocesamiento del Movimiento Ocular , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/fisiopatología
7.
Eur J Neurosci ; 57(10): 1723-1735, 2023 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36967647

RESUMEN

Gaze following is a major element of non-verbal communication and important for successful social interactions. Human gaze following is a fast and almost reflex-like behaviour, yet it can be volitionally controlled and suppressed to some extent if inappropriate or unnecessary, given the social context. In order to identify the neural basis of the cognitive control of gaze following, we carried out an event-related fMRI experiment, in which human subjects' eye movements were tracked while they were exposed to gaze cues in two distinct contexts: A baseline gaze following condition in which subjects were instructed to use gaze cues to shift their attention to a gazed-at spatial target and a control condition in which the subjects were required to ignore the gaze cue and instead to shift their attention to a distinct spatial target to be selected based on a colour mapping rule, requiring the suppression of gaze following. We could identify a suppression-related blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) response in a frontoparietal network comprising dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), the anterior insula, precuneus, and posterior parietal cortex (PPC). These findings suggest that overexcitation of frontoparietal circuits in turn suppressing the gaze following patch might be a potential cause of gaze following deficits in clinical populations.


Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular , Lóbulo Frontal , Red Nerviosa , Lóbulo Parietal , Volición , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Humanos , Señales (Psicología) , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Volición/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Femenino
8.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 30(1): 235-249, 2023 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35915381

RESUMEN

Human perceptual learning, experience-induced gains in sensory discrimination, typically yields long-term performance improvements. Recent research revealed long-lasting transfer at the untrained location enabled by feature-based attention (FBA), reminiscent of its global effect (Hung & Carrasco, Scientific Reports, 11(1), 13914, (2021)). Visual Perceptual Learning (VPL) is typically studied while observers maintain fixation, but the role of fixational eye movements is unknown. Microsaccades - the largest of fixational eye movements - provide a continuous, online, physiological measure from the oculomotor system that reveals dynamic processing, which is unavailable from behavioral measures alone. We investigated whether and how microsaccades change after training in an orientation discrimination task. For human observers trained with or without FBA, microsaccade rates were significantly reduced during the response window in both trained and untrained locations and orientations. Critically, consistent with long-term training benefits, this microsaccade-rate reduction persisted over a year. Furthermore, microsaccades were biased toward the target location prior to stimulus onset and were more suppressed for incorrect than correct trials after observers' responses. These findings reveal that fixational eye movements and VPL are tightly coupled and that learning-induced microsaccade changes are long lasting. Thus, microsaccades reflect functional dynamics of the oculomotor system during information encoding, maintenance and readout, and may serve as a reliable long-term physiological correlate in VPL.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Aprendizaje , Movimientos Sacádicos , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Atención/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Aprendizaje Espacial/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
9.
Rev. bras. oftalmol ; 82: e0015, 2023. tab, graf
Artículo en Inglés | LILACS | ID: biblio-1431667

RESUMEN

ABSTRACT Objective: To evaluate if participants, subjected to whole-body vibration, two different types of media (paper versus tablet) and two lighting environments (fluorescent versus LED), present a difference in eye-movement parameters during reading tasks. Methods: Fourteen adults silently read two different texts in each one of the eight randomized testing conditions (whole-body vibration versus media versus lighting), resulting in 16 different texts read per individual. Whole-body vibration was applied in the vertical direction, 5Hz and 0.8 m/s2 root-mean-square amplitude, a condition similar to those experienced by forklift truck drivers. Participants were in a sitting position with a backrest. An eye-tracker evaluated the eye-movements during the reading task. Results: Whole-body vibration significantly reduced the number of ocular fixations, and cross-correlation; and increased the reading efficiency, fixation duration, directional attack, and binocular anomalies. Neither the type of media nor the lighting environment interfered significantly with the eye-movements, both in situations with and without vibration. Conclusion: The results indicate that whole-body vibration interfered in the eye-movements during the reading task. This may impose a difficulty to process the visual information and to synchronously coordinate the binocular movements under vibration environments.


RESUMO Objetivo: Avaliar se participantes submetidos à vibração de corpo inteiro, a dois tipos diferentes de mídia (papel versus tablet) e a dois ambientes de iluminação (fluorescente versus LED) apresentam diferença nos parâmetros de movimento dos olhos durante tarefas de leitura. Métodos: Quatorze adultos leram silenciosamente dois textos diferentes em cada uma das oito condições de teste (vibração de corpo inteiro versus mídia versus iluminação), de forma aleatória, resultando em 16 textos diferentes lidos por indivíduo. A vibração de corpo inteiro foi aplicada no sentido vertical, com amplitude de 5Hz e 0,8m/s² da raiz do valor quadrático médio, em condição semelhante às vivenciadas pelos motoristas de empilhadeiras. Os participantes permaneceram em postura sentada com encosto. Um rastreador ocular avaliou os movimentos oculares durante a leitura. Resultados: A vibração de corpo inteiro reduziu significativamente o número de fixações oculares e a correlação cruzada entre os olhos e aumentou a eficiência de leitura, duração da fixação, ataque direcional e anomalias binoculares. Nem o tipo de mídia nem as condições de ambientes de iluminação interferiram significativamente nos movimentos oculares, tanto em situações com ou sem vibração. Conclusão: Os resultados indicam que a Vibração de Corpo Inteiro pode interferir nos movimentos oculares durante a leitura. Isso pode impor uma dificuldade no processamento da informação visual e na coordenação síncrona dos movimentos binoculares em ambientes de vibração.


Asunto(s)
Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Lectura , Vibración , Iluminación , Computadoras de Mano , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Estudios Prospectivos , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Luz
10.
Curr Alzheimer Res ; 19(3): 212-222, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35422217

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Eye movement patterns during reading are well defined and documented. Each eye movement ends up in a fixation point, which allows the brain to process the incoming information and program the following saccade. In this work, we investigated whether eye movement alterations during a reading task might be already present in middle-aged, cognitively normal offspring of late-onset Alzheimer's disease (O-LOAD). METHODS: 18 O-LOAD and 18 age-matched healthy individuals with no family history of LOAD participated in the study. Participants were seated in front of a 20-inch LCD monitor, and single sentences were presented on it. Eye movements were recorded with an eye tracker with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. RESULTS: Analysis of eye movements during reading revealed that O-LOAD displayed more fixations, shorter saccades, and shorter fixation durations than controls. CONCLUSION: The present study shows that O-LOAD experienced alterations in their eye movements during reading. O-LOAD eye movement behavior could be considered an initial sign of oculomotor impairment. Hence, the evaluation of eye movement during reading might be a useful tool for monitoring well-defined cognitive resources.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Hijo de Padres Discapacitados , Trastornos de la Motilidad Ocular , Lectura , Edad de Inicio , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/epidemiología , Hijo de Padres Discapacitados/estadística & datos numéricos , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Trastornos de la Motilidad Ocular/fisiopatología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología
11.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(8)2022 02 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35169072

RESUMEN

Humans make sense of the world by organizing things into categories. When and how does this process begin? We investigated whether real-world object categories that spontaneously emerge in the first months of life match categorical representations of objects in the human visual cortex. Using eye tracking, we measured the differential looking time of 4-, 10-, and 19-mo-olds as they looked at pairs of pictures belonging to eight animate or inanimate categories (human/nonhuman, faces/bodies, real-world size big/small, natural/artificial). Taking infants' looking times as a measure of similarity, for each age group, we defined a representational space where each object was defined in relation to others of the same or of a different category. This space was compared with hypothesis-based and functional MRI-based models of visual object categorization in the adults' visual cortex. Analyses across different age groups showed that, as infants grow older, their looking behavior matches neural representations in ever-larger portions of the adult visual cortex, suggesting progressive recruitment and integration of more and more feature spaces distributed over the visual cortex. Moreover, the results characterize infants' visual categorization as an incremental process with two milestones. Between 4 and 10 mo, visual exploration guided by saliency gives way to an organization according to the animate-inanimate distinction. Between 10 and 19 mo, a category spurt leads toward a mature organization. We propose that these changes underlie the coupling between seeing and thinking in the developing mind.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Tecnología de Seguimiento Ocular , Femenino , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Humanos , Lactante , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Pensamiento/fisiología , Visión Ocular/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología
12.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 63(2): 33, 2022 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35212720

RESUMEN

Purpose: Patients with amblyopia are known to have fixation instability, which arises from alteration of physiologic fixation eye movements (FEMs) and nystagmus. We assessed the effects of monocular, binocular, and dichoptic viewing on FEMs and eye alignment in patients with and without fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN). Methods: Thirty-four patients with amblyopia and seven healthy controls were recruited for this study. Eye movements were recorded using infrared video-oculography during (1) fellow eye viewing (FEV), (2) amblyopic eye viewing (AEV), (3) both eye viewing (BEV), and (4) dichoptic viewing (DcV) at varying fellow eye (FE) contrasts. The patients were classified per the clinical type of amblyopia and FEM waveforms into those without nystagmus, those with nystagmus with and without FMN. Fixational saccades and intersaccadic drifts, quick and slow phases of nystagmus, and bivariate contour ellipse area were analyzed in the FE and amblyopic eye (AE). Results: We found that FEMs are differentially affected with increased amplitude of quick phases of FMN observed during AEV than BEV and during DcV at lower FE contrasts. Increased fixation instability was seen in anisometropic patients at lower FE contrasts. Incomitance of eye misalignment was seen with the greatest increase during FEV. Strabismic/mixed amblyopia patients without FMN were more likely to demonstrate a fixation switch where the AE attends to the target during DcV than patients with FMN. Conclusions: Our findings suggest that FEM abnormalities modulate with different viewing conditions as used in various amblyopia therapies. Increased FEM abnormalities could affect the visual function deficits and may have treatment implications.


Asunto(s)
Ambliopía/fisiopatología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Nistagmo Patológico/fisiopatología , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Grabación en Video , Agudeza Visual
13.
Cell Rep ; 38(1): 110198, 2022 01 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34986350

RESUMEN

Goal-directed behavior requires identifying objects in the environment that can satisfy internal needs and executing actions to obtain those objects. The current study examines ventral and dorsal corticostriatal circuits that support complementary aspects of goal-directed behavior. We analyze activity from the amygdala, ventral striatum, orbitofrontal cortex, and lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) while monkeys perform a three-armed bandit task. Information about chosen stimuli and their value is primarily encoded in the amygdala, ventral striatum, and orbitofrontal cortex, while the spatial information is primarily encoded in the LPFC. Before the options are presented, information about the to-be-chosen stimulus is represented in the amygdala, ventral striatum, and orbitofrontal cortex; at the time of choice, the information is passed to the LPFC to direct a saccade. Thus, learned value information specifying behavioral goals is maintained throughout the ventral corticostriatal circuit, and it is routed through the dorsal circuit at the time actions are selected.


Asunto(s)
Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiología , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Estriado Ventral/fisiología , Animales , Condicionamiento Operante/fisiología , Objetivos , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Recompensa , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología
14.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 1390, 2022 01 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35082319

RESUMEN

Pupillometry has become a standard measure for assessing arousal state. However, environmental factors such as luminance, a primary dictator of pupillary responses, often vary across studies. To what degree does luminance interact with arousal-driven pupillary changes? Here, we parametrically assessed luminance-driven pupillary responses across a wide-range of luminances, while concurrently manipulating cognitive arousal using auditory math problems of varying difficulty. At the group-level, our results revealed that the modulatory effect of cognitive arousal on pupil size interacts multiplicatively with luminance, with the largest effects occurring at low and mid-luminances. However, at the level of individuals, there were qualitatively distinct individual differences in the modulatory effect of cognitive arousal on luminance-driven pupillary responses. Our findings suggest that pupillometry as a measure for assessing arousal requires more careful consideration: there are ranges of luminance levels that are more ideal in observing pupillary differences between arousal conditions than others.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Nivel de Alerta/efectos de la radiación , Luz , Pupila/fisiología , Pupila/efectos de la radiación , Visión Ocular/fisiología , Visión Ocular/efectos de la radiación , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Cognición/fisiología , Cognición/efectos de la radiación , Femenino , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/efectos de la radiación , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Frecuencia Cardíaca/efectos de la radiación , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Pantalla , Adulto Joven
15.
J Neurosci ; 42(5): 789-803, 2022 02 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34880121

RESUMEN

Omnipause neurons (OPNs) in the nucleus raphe interpositus have tonic activity while the eyes are stationary ("fixation") but stop firing immediately before and during saccades. To locate the source of suppression, we analyzed synaptic inputs from the rostral and caudal superior colliculi (SCs) to OPNs by using intracellular recording and staining, and investigated pathways transmitting the inputs in anesthetized cats of both sexes. Electrophysiologically or morphologically identified OPNs received monosynaptic excitation from the rostral SCs with contralateral dominance, and received disynaptic inhibition from the caudal SCs with ipsilateral dominance. Cutting the tectoreticular tract transversely between the contralateral OPN and inhibitory burst neuron (IBN) regions eliminated inhibition from the caudal SCs, but not excitation from the rostral SCs in OPNs. In contrast, a midline section between IBN regions eliminated disynaptic inhibition in OPNs from the caudal SCs but did not affect the monosynaptic excitation from the rostral SCs. Stimulation of the contralateral IBN region evoked monosynaptic inhibition in OPNs, which was facilitated by preconditioning SC stimulation. Three-dimensional reconstruction of HRP-stained cells revealed that individual OPNs have axons that terminate in the opposite IBN area, while individual IBNs have axon collaterals to the opposite OPN area. These results show that there are differences in the neural circuit from the rostral and caudal SCs to the brainstem premotor circuitry and that IBNs suppress OPNs immediately before and during saccades. Thus, the IBNs, which are activated by caudal SC saccade neurons, shut down OPN firing and help to trigger saccades and suppress ("latch") OPN activity during saccades.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Saccades are the fastest eye movements to redirect gaze to an object of interest and bring its image on the fovea for fixation. Burst neurons (BNs) and omnipause neurons (OPNs) which behave reciprocally in the brainstem, are important for saccade generation and fixation. This study investigated unsolved important questions about where these neurons receive command signals and how they interact for initiating saccades from visual fixation. The results show that the rostral superior colliculi (SCs) excite OPNs monosynaptically for fixation, whereas the caudal SCs monosynaptically excite inhibitory BNs, which then directly inhibit OPNs for the initiation of saccades. This inhibition from the caudal SCs may account for the omnipause behavior of OPNs for initiation and maintenance of saccades in all directions.


Asunto(s)
Tronco Encefálico/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Potenciales Sinápticos/fisiología , Animales , Gatos , Femenino , Potenciales Postsinápticos Inhibidores/fisiología , Masculino , Microelectrodos , Colículos Superiores/fisiología
16.
J Neurosci ; 42(4): 670-681, 2022 01 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34862190

RESUMEN

To competently navigate the world, individuals must flexibly balance distinct aspects of social gaze, orienting toward others and inhibiting orienting responses, depending on the context. These behaviors are often disrupted amongst patient populations treated with serotonergic drugs. However, those in the field lack a clear understanding of how the serotonergic system mediates social orienting and inhibiting behaviors. Here, we tested how increasing central concentrations of serotonin with the direct precursor 5-hydroxytryptophan (5-HTP) would modulate the ability of rhesus macaques (both sexes) to use eye movements to flexibly orient to, or inhibit orienting to, faces. Systemic administrations of 5-HTP effectively increased central serotonin levels and impaired flexible orientation and inhibition. Critically, 5-HTP selectively impaired the ability of monkeys to inhibit orienting to face images, whereas it similarly impaired orienting to face and control images. 5-HTP also caused monkeys to perseverate on their gaze responses, making them worse at flexibly switching between orienting and inhibiting behaviors. Furthermore, the effects of 5-HTP on performance correlated with a constriction of the pupil, an increased time to initiate trials, and an increased reaction time, suggesting that the disruptive effects of 5-HTP on social gaze behaviors are likely driven by a downregulation of arousal and motivational states. Together, these findings provide causal evidence for a modulatory relationship between 5-HTP and social gaze behaviors in nonhuman primates and offer translational insights for the role of the serotonergic system in social gaze.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Behavioral changes arising from pharmacological agents that target serotonergic functions are complex and difficult to predict. Here, we examined the causal impacts of administering the direct precursor of serotonin, 5-HTP, on orienting and inhibiting social gaze in nonhuman primates. 5-HTP increased central concentrations of serotonin and selectively impaired the ability of monkeys to inhibit orienting to faces while similarly impairing the ability of monkeys to orient to face and control images. These behavioral gaze impairments were systematically associated with a downregulation of arousal and motivational states, indexed by pupil constriction, increased time to initiate trials, and increased reaction time. These findings provide a causal link between 5-HTP and social gaze behaviors in nonhuman primates and provide translational insights about serotonergic interventions.


Asunto(s)
5-Hidroxitriptófano/administración & dosificación , 5-Hidroxitriptófano/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Fijación Ocular/efectos de los fármacos , Orientación/efectos de los fármacos , Serotonina/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Interacción Social/efectos de los fármacos , Animales , Femenino , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Inyecciones Intramusculares , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Orientación/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Primates
17.
Psychol Res ; 86(5): 1578-1590, 2022 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34374844

RESUMEN

This study aimed to investigate the facilitatory versus inhibitory effects of dynamic non-predictive central cues presented in a realistic environment. Realistic human-avatars initiated eye contact and then dynamically looked to the left, right or centre of a table. A moving stick served as a non-social control cue and participants localised (Experiment 1) or discriminated (Experiment 2) a contextually relevant target (teapot/teacup). The cues movement took 500 ms and stimulus onset asynchronies (SOA, 150 ms/300 ms/500 ms/1000 ms) were measured from movement initiation. Similar cuing effects were seen for the social avatar and non-social stick cue across tasks. Results showed facilitatory processes without inhibition, though there was some variation by SOA and task. This is the first time facilitatory versus inhibitory processes have been directly investigated where eye contact is initiated prior to gaze shift. These dynamic stimuli allow a better understanding of how attention might be cued in more realistic environments.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Señales (Psicología) , Fijación Ocular , Atención/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica
18.
PLoS One ; 16(12): e0261434, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34914809

RESUMEN

Attention is considered to be a critical part of the sexual response cycle, and researchers have differentiated between the roles of initial (involuntary) and subsequent (voluntary) attention paid to sexual stimuli as part of the facilitation of sexual arousal. Prior studies using eye-tracking methodologies have shown differing initial attention patterns to erotic stimuli between men and women, as well as between individuals of different sexual orientations. No study has directly compared initial attention to sexual stimuli in asexual individuals, defined by their lack of sexual attraction, to women with Sexual Interest/Arousal Disorder (SIAD), a disorder characterized by a reduced or absent interest in sex coupled with significant personal distress. The current study tested differences in the initial attention patterns of 29 asexual individuals (Mage = 26.56, SD = 4.80) and 25 heterosexual women with SIAD (Mage = 27.52, SD = 4.87), using eye-tracking. Participants were presented with sexual and neutral stimuli, and their initial eye movements and initial fixations to both image types and areas of erotic contact within sexual images were recorded. Mixed-model ANOVAs and t-tests were used to compare the two groups on the speed with which their initial fixations occurred, the duration of their initial fixations, and the proportion of initial fixations made to sexual stimuli. On two indices of initial attention, women with SIAD displayed an initial attention preference for sexual stimuli over neutral stimuli compared to asexual participants. This study adds to a growing literature on the distinction between asexuality and SIAD, indicating that differences in early attention may be a feature that differentiates the groups.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Identidad de Género , Excitación Sexual , Disfunciones Sexuales Psicológicas/psicología , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Depresión/psicología , Literatura Erótica , Tecnología de Seguimiento Ocular , Femenino , Finlandia , Humanos , Masculino , Conducta Sexual/psicología
19.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 118(52)2021 12 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34933998

RESUMEN

The learning of first object names is deemed a hard problem due to the uncertainty inherent in mapping a heard name to the intended referent in a cluttered and variable world. However, human infants readily solve this problem. Despite considerable theoretical discussion, relatively little is known about the uncertainty infants face in the real world. We used head-mounted eye tracking during parent-infant toy play and quantified the uncertainty by measuring the distribution of infant attention to the potential referents when a parent named both familiar and unfamiliar toy objects. The results show that infant gaze upon hearing an object name is often directed to a single referent which is equally likely to be a wrong competitor or the intended target. This bimodal gaze distribution clarifies and redefines the uncertainty problem and constrains possible solutions.


Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Incertidumbre , Humanos , Lactante
20.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 62(15): 24, 2021 12 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34935881

RESUMEN

Purpose: To investigate translatory movement during the lateral gaze in patients with horizontal strabismus using magnetic resonance imaging. Methods: Patients with esotropia or exotropia and normal controls underwent orbital magnetic resonance imaging during the central gaze and lateral gaze at 40°. The position of the static tissues was superimposed three-dimensionally for all gazes using a self-developed software, allowing the analysis of the net eyeball movement. Then, the eyeball centroid coordinates were extracted for each gaze, and the distance and direction of centroid movement from the central to lateral gaze were calculated. Results: The mean distance ± standard deviation of the centroid movement was 1.0 ± 0.5 mm during abduction in the exotropia group, which was significantly longer than that in the esotropia (0.6 ± 0.3 mm; P = 0.003) and control (0.7 ± 0.2 mm; P = 0.002) groups. Conversely, the centroid moved farther in the esotropia group (0.9 ± 0.3 mm) than the exotropia (0.6 ± 0.3 mm; P = 0.005) and control (0.7 ± 0.2 mm; P = 0.023) groups during adduction. Posterior translation during abduction was longer in the exotropia group (-0.8 ± 0.3 mm) compared with the esotropia (-0.5 ± 0.3 mm; P = 0.017) and control (-0.4 ± 0.3 mm; P = 0.001) groups, whereas that during adduction was longer in the esotropia group (-0.4 ± 0.4 mm) than the exotropia (-0.1 ± 0.2 mm; P = 0.033) and control (-0.1 ± 0.2 mm; P = 0.026) groups. Conclusions: During abduction, more translatory movement occurred in the exotropia group, whereas the centroid moved farther in the esotropia group during adduction. The translatory movement difference between both strabismus groups implies that there is a difference in biomechanics among the types of strabismus.


Asunto(s)
Esotropía/fisiopatología , Exotropía/fisiopatología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Músculos Oculomotores/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Ojo/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Humanos , Imagenología Tridimensional , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Músculos Oculomotores/diagnóstico por imagen , Estudios Prospectivos , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA